Slide 1

Slide 1 text

1 Gender stereotypes are reflected in the distributional structure of 25 languages Molly Lewis Carnegie Mellon University Computational Social Science Workshop @ U Chicago 21 May 2020

Slide 2

Slide 2 text

Gender disparities are pervasive e.g., STEM fields (Huang, et al., 2020)

Slide 3

Slide 3 text

3 Stereotypes develop in childhood… % Male scientist drawn Age (years) (Miller et al, 2018) 0 100 50

Slide 4

Slide 4 text

Where do stereotypes come from? Learn a lot from language: The earth is round. Mongolia is really cold. Octopi have three hearts. Boys are better at math than girls. What about more implicit messages in language? 4

Slide 5

Slide 5 text

Semantic information from word co-occurrences Distributional semantics: Semantic similarity between two words A and B is a function of the similarity of the linguistic contexts in which they appear. Sam ate the red apple near the red barn... Sam ate the red apple near barn Sam 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 ate 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 the 0 1 0 2 0 1 0 red 0 0 2 0 1 0 1 apple 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 near 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 barn 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 . . . ... 5

Slide 6

Slide 6 text

Reducing dimensionality of co-occurrence statistics extracts semantic information • Represent all words from a corpus within a k- dimensional space • Preserving distances between words in their local contexts • Similar to factor analysis 6 k = 3 (Kozlowski, Taddy, & Evans, 2019)

Slide 7

Slide 7 text

Distributional models as learning models 7 HAL (Lund & Burgess, 1996) LSA (Landauer & Dumais, 1997) Word2vec (Mikolov, Chen, Corrado, & Dean, 2013) GloVe (Pennington, Socher, & Manning, 2014) … Cognitive Theory (Cognitive Science) Solving language tasks (Machine Learning)

Slide 8

Slide 8 text

Humans are good at learning statistics • Co-occurrence statistics to identify words (Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996) • Co-occurrence statistics to identify meanings (Smith & Yu, 2008) • Co-occurrence statistics in the visual domain (Kirkham, Slemmer, & Johnson, 2002) • Distributional statistics about everyday events (Griffiths & Tenenbaum, 2006) 8

Slide 9

Slide 9 text

Do humans learn social stereotypes by tracking distributional statistics in language? 9

Slide 10

Slide 10 text

Gender stereotype 10 Men - career Women - family

Slide 11

Slide 11 text

Implicit Association Test (IAT) Categories X = {man, male, he, him, boy} Y = {woman, female, she, her, girl} Attributes A = {career, salary, office, business, professional} B = {family, home, parents, children, cousins} Participants slower for incongruent mapping (right), suggesting bias to associate men with career. man career woman family compare reaction time man career woman family 11

Slide 12

Slide 12 text

Quantifying the implicit bias: IAT effect size Effect size = mean RT in incongruent condition – mean RT in congruent condition 12 Bigger effect size -> bigger bias

Slide 13

Slide 13 text

(male− family) −0.04 −0.02 0.00 0.02 (male− career) Implicit psychological gender bias by country Implicit gender bias by country (male− family) −0.04 −0.02 0.00 0.02 (male− career) N = 764,520 participants (Project Implicit: Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002) https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/ 13

Slide 14

Slide 14 text

(male− family) −0.04 −0.02 0.00 0.02 (male− career) Implicit psychological gender bias by country Does bias in language predict bias in IAT? Psychological measure (IAT) Language measure (word-occurrences) Word embedding models trained on the 25 languages spoken by participants in the sample of countries 14

Slide 15

Slide 15 text

+ Measure gender bias from human judgements (≈ 4,500 words; Scott et al., 2018) Word embedding model trained on corpus of movie and TV subtitles in English (Lison & Tiedemann, 2016; Van Paridon & Thompson, in prep.). Quantify gender association in semantic space as cosine distance Validating distributional statistics as encoding gender semantics + + + + + he son male boy his him brother ”home” + + man + + + + + hers daughter female girl she her sister + + woman + 15 Very masculine Very feminine Rate the gender association of the word “home”

Slide 16

Slide 16 text

16 r = 0.63 1 (male) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (female) −0.15 (male) −.1 −.05 0.0 .05 .1 .15 (female) Linguistic Gender Association Human Judgement of Gender Association Replicated on model trained on English Wikipedia (r = .59)

Slide 17

Slide 17 text

Implicit Association Test (IAT) Categories X = {man, male, he, him, boy} Y = {woman, female, she, her, girl} Attributes A = {career, salary, office, business, professional} B = {family, home, parents, children, cousins} man career woman family compare reaction time man career woman family …based on word co-occurrences (using the same method as Caliskan, Bryson, & Narayanan, 2017) compare distance in semantic space 17

Slide 18

Slide 18 text

Language IAT effect size 18 Categories X* = {man, male, he, him, boy} Y* = {woman, female, she, her, girl} Attributes A* = {career, salary, office, business, professional} B* = {family, home, parents, children, cousins} * translated into each language

Slide 19

Slide 19 text

Arabic Danish German English Spanish Persian Finnish French Hebrew Hindi Croatian Indonesian Italian Japanese Korean Malay Dutch Norwegian Polish Portuguese Romanian Swedish Filipino Turkish Mandarin r = 0.48 −.075 (weaker) −.05 −.025 0 .025 .05 (stronger) −.25 (weaker) 0 .25 .5 .75 1 (stronger) Language Male−Career Association (effect size) Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) N participants 1,000 10,000 100,000 Implicit and Linguistic Male−Career Association a Athletic−Intelligent Avoiding−Approaching Career−Family Cold−Hot Friends−Family Helpers−Leaders Innocence−Wisdom Jocks−Nerd Lawyers−Po Money−Love Defense−E Labor−Management Protein−Carbs. Punishment−Forgiveness Rebellious−Conforming Security−Freedom Skeptical−Tru State−Church Tall−Short Team−Individual Technology−Nature Urban−Rural ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● −.2 (US Greater) −.1 0 .1 .2 .3 (UK Greater) −1.5 (US Greater) −1 −.5 0 Language Association (effect size) Implicit Association Difference (residualized effect size) Implicit and Linguistic Associa in British and American Partic b

Slide 20

Slide 20 text

What about gender information encoded more explicitly in language? Grammatical gender “enfermero” (nurse-MASC) “enfermera” (nurse-FEM) 20 Lexicalized gender “waiter” (waiter-MASC) “waitress” (waiter-FEM)

Slide 21

Slide 21 text

Explicit linguistic gender and implicit bias • 12/25 languages encoded gender grammatical • Languages that encoded gender grammatically did not have more implicit bias. (Mdiff = 0.01 [-0.01, 0.03]) • In contrast, languages with more lexicalized gender forms have more implicit bias. 21 Romanian Portuguese German Spanish Hebrew Polish Croatian Italian French Dutch Danish Swedish Norwegian English Japanese Korean Mandarin Finnish Hindi Turkish Arabic Persian Indonesian Malay Filipino r = 0.57 −.075 (weaker) −.05 −.025 0 .025 .05 (stronger) 0.00 0.25 0.50 .75 1.00 Prop. Gender−Specific Occupation Terms Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) Implicit Male−Career Association and Gendered Occupation Terms a ( Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) b

Slide 22

Slide 22 text

Summary Statistical associations in language reflect human judgements of gender associations Speakers of languages with stronger statistical associations between men-career and women- family tend to have a stronger bias when measured via the IAT Speakers of languages with more lexicalized gender distinctions (but not grammatical gender) tend to have a stronger bias in the IAT. 22 Romanian Portuguese German Spanish Hebrew Polish Croatian Italian French Dutch Danish Swedish Norwegian English Japanese Korean Mandarin Finnish Hindi Turkish Arabic Persian Indonesian Malay Filipino r = 0.57 −.075 (weaker) −.05 −.025 0 .025 .05 (stronger) 0.00 0.25 0.50 .75 1.00 Prop. Gender−Specific Occupation Terms Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) Implicit Male−Career Association and Gendered Occupation Terms a Romanian Portuguese German Spanish Hebrew Polish Croatian Italian French Dutch Danish Swedish Norwegian English Japanese Korean Mandarin Finnish Hindi Turkish Arabic Persian Indonesian Malay Filipino r = 0.49 −.075 (weaker) −.05 −.025 0 .025 .05 (stronger) 0 (netural) .025 .05 .075 .1 (gendered) Genderness of Occupation Terms Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) N participants 1,000 10,000 100,000 Implicit Male−Career Association and Gender Associations for Occupation Terms b Arabic Danish German English Spanish Persian Finnish French Hebrew Hindi Croatian Indonesian Italian Japanese Korean Malay Dutch Norwegian Polish Portuguese Romanian Swedish Filipino Turkish Mandarin r = 0.48 −.075 (weaker) −.05 −.025 0 .025 .05 (stronger) −.25 (weaker) 0 .25 .5 .75 1 (stronger) Language Male−Career Association (effect size) Implicit Male−Career Association (residualized effect size) N participants 1,000 10,000 100,000 Implicit and Linguistic Male−Career Association a Athletic−Intelligent Avoiding−Approaching Career−Family Chaos−Order Cold−Hot Determinism−Free Will Friends−Family Helpers−Leaders Innocence−Wisdom Jocks−Nerds Lawyers−Politicians Money−Love Defense−Education Labor−Management Poor−Rich Private−Public Protein−Carbs. Punishment−Forgiveness Rebellious−Conforming Rich−Beautiful Security−Freedom Skeptical−Trusting Speed−Accuracy Stable−Flexible State−Church Tall−Short Team−Individual Technology−Nature Tradition−Progress Urban−Rural Winter−Summer ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● ● r = 0.3 −.2 (US Greater) −.1 0 .1 .2 .3 (UK Greater) −1.5 (US Greater) −1 −.5 0 .5 1 (UK Greater) Language Association Difference (effect size) Implicit Association Difference (residualized effect size) Implicit and Linguistic Associations in British and American Participants b r = 0.63 1 (male) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (female) −0.15 (male) −.1 −.05 0.0 .05 .1 .15 (female) Linguistic Gender Association Human Judgement of Gender Association

Slide 23

Slide 23 text

Do humans learn social stereotypes by tracking distributional statistics in language? 23 Evidence for a correspondence between human semantic knowledge and distributional statistics (necessary but not sufficient) Testing the causal question and other implications

Slide 24

Slide 24 text

Is the link causal? • All the evidence I’ve presented so far is correlational • Likely bi-directional • What kind of evidence might we bring to bear on this? • Longitudinal analyses: e.g., testing whether changes in language statistics predict or follow changes in measured implicit associations (Greenwald, 2017; Charlesworth & Banaji, 2019) • Quasi-experimental tests: e.g., measuring implicit associations in bilinguals using stimuli in languages that embed different linguistic associations • Experimental designs: measure the effect of manipulating language statistics on people’s implicit associations. 24 Distributional statistics Human semantic representations

Slide 25

Slide 25 text

Gender bias in children’s books If biases are learned from language, expect them to be present in the input to people who are learning the biases (i.e. children) 25 −0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 male−career male−math (vs. language) male−math (vs. art) Bias Type Language IAT effect size Languag Wikipe Adult F Childre Language IAT Bias in Different Corpora

Slide 26

Slide 26 text

Language gender bias and other causal forces contributing to gender differences

Slide 27

Slide 27 text

Students to report on how easy they thought it would be for them to: • recognize the science question that underlies a newspaper report on a health issue • explain why earthquakes occur more frequently in some areas than in others • describe the role of antibiotics in the treatment of disease • etc. Language gender bias and other causal forces contributing to gender differences

Slide 28

Slide 28 text

Do humans learn social stereotypes by tracking distributional statistics in language? • Evidence for a close correspondence between human semantic knowledge and distributional statistics in the case of a particular stereotype: men-career; women-family • Consistent with the idea that language is playing a causal role in shaping social stereotypes. • Strongly correlated with other hypothesized “psychological” causal factors • Additional work needs to be done to more directly test causality, and relationship to other causal forces • Suggests that intervening on language input could change biases

Slide 29

Slide 29 text

Thanks! Gary Lupyan (U. of Wisconsin-Madison)